×

We use cookies to help make LingQ better. By visiting the site, you agree to our cookie policy.

image

TED Talks, Tali Sharot | The optimism bias (1)

Tali Sharot | The optimism bias (1)

I'm going to talk to you about optimism -- or more precisely, the optimism bias. It's a cognitive illusion that we've been studying in my lab for the past few years, and 80 percent of us have it. It's our tendency to overestimate our likelihood of experiencing good events in our lives and underestimate our likelihood of experiencing bad events. So we underestimate our likelihood of suffering from cancer, being in a car accident. We overestimate our longevity, our career prospects. In short, we're more optimistic than realistic, but we are oblivious to the fact. Take marriage for example. In the Western world, divorce rates are about 40 percent. That means that out of five married couples, two will end up splitting their assets. But when you ask newlyweds about their own likelihood of divorce, they estimate it at zero percent. And even divorce lawyers, who should really know better, hugely underestimate their own likelihood of divorce. So it turns out that optimists are not less likely to divorce, but they are more likely to remarry. In the words of Samuel Johnson, "Remarriage is the triumph of hope over experience." So if we're married, we're more likely to have kids. And we all think our kids will be especially talented. This, by the way, is my two-year-old nephew, Guy. And I just want to make it absolutely clear that he's a really bad example of the optimism bias, because he is in fact uniquely talented. And I'm not alone. Out of four British people, three said that they were optimistic about the future of their own families. That's 75 percent. But only 30 percent said that they thought families in general are doing better than a few generations ago. And this is a really important point, because we're optimistic about ourselves, we're optimistic about our kids,we're optimistic about our families, but we're not so optimistic about the guy sitting next to us, and we're somewhat pessimistic about the fate of our fellow citizens and the fate of our country. But private optimism about our own personal future remains persistent. And it doesn't mean that we think things will magically turn out okay, but rather that we have the unique ability to make it so. Now I'm a scientist, I do experiments. So to show you what I mean, I'm going to do an experiment here with you. So I'm going to give you a list of abilities and characteristics, and I want you to think for each of these abilities where you stand relative to the rest of the population. The first one is getting along well with others. Who here believes they're at the bottom 25 percent? Okay, that's about 10 people out of 1,500. Who believes they're at the top 25 percent? That's most of us here. Okay, now do the same for your driving ability. How interesting are you? How attractive are you? How honest are you? And finally, how modest are you? So most of us put ourselves above average on most of these abilities. Now this is statistically impossible. We can't all be better than everyone else. (Laughter) But if we believe we're better than the other guy, well that means that we're more likely to get that promotion, to remain married, because we're more social, more interesting. And it's a global phenomenon. The optimism bias has been observed in many different countries -- in Western cultures, in non-Western cultures, in females and males, in kids, in the elderly. It's quite widespread. But the question is, is it good for us? So some people say no. Some people say the secret to happiness is low expectations. I think the logic goes something like this: If we don't expect greatness, if we don't expect to find love and be healthy and successful, well we're not going to be disappointed when these things don't happen.And if we're not disappointed when good things don't happen, and we're pleasantly surprised when they do,we will be happy. So it's a very good theory, but it turns out to be wrong for three reasons. Number one: Whatever happens, whether you succeed or you fail, people with high expectations always feel better. Because how we feel when we get dumped or win employee of the month depends on how we interpret that event. The psychologists Margaret Marshall and John Brown studied students with high and low expectations. And they found that when people with high expectations succeed, they attribute that success to their own traits. "I'm a genius, therefore I got an A, therefore I'll get an A again and again in the future. " When they failed, it wasn't because they were dumb, but because the exam just happened to be unfair. Next time they will do better. People with low expectations do the opposite. So when they failed it was because they were dumb,and when they succeeded it was because the exam just happened to be really easy. Next time reality would catch up with them. So they felt worse. Number two: Regardless of the outcome, the pure act of anticipation makes us happy. The behavioral economist George Lowenstein asked students in his university to imagine getting a passionate kiss from a celebrity, any celebrity. Then he said, "How much are you willing to pay to get a kiss from a celebrity if the kiss was delivered immediately, in three hours, in 24 hours, in three days, in one year, in 10 years? He found that the students were willing to pay the most not to get a kiss immediately, but to get a kiss in three days. They were willing to pay extra in order to wait. Now they weren't willing to wait a year or 10 years; no one wants an aging celebrity. But three days seemed to be the optimum amount. So why is that? Well if you get the kiss now, it's over and done with. But if you get the kiss in three days, well that's three days of jittery anticipation, the thrill of the wait. The students wanted that time to imagine where is it going to happen, how is it going to happen. Anticipation made them happy. This is, by the way, why people prefer Friday to Sunday. It's a really curious fact, because Friday is a day of work and Sunday is a day of pleasure, so you'd assume that people will prefer Sunday, but they don't. It's not because they really, really like being in the office and they can't stand strolling in the park or having a lazy brunch. We know that, because when you ask people about their ultimate favorite day of the week, surprise, surprise, Saturday comes in at first, then Friday, then Sunday. People prefer Friday because Friday brings with it the anticipation of the weekend ahead, all the plans that you have. On Sunday, the only thing you can look forward to is the work week. So optimists are people who expect more kisses in their future, more strolls in the park. And that anticipation enhances their wellbeing. In fact, without the optimism bias, we would all be slightly depressed. People with mild depression, they don't have a bias when they look into the future. They're actually more realistic than healthy individuals. But individuals with severe depression, they have a pessimistic bias. So they tend to expect the future to be worse than it ends up being. So optimism changes subjective reality. The way we expect the world to be changes the way we see it. But it also changes objective reality. It acts as a self-fulfilling prophecy. And that is the third reason why lowering your expectations will not make you happy. Controlled experiments have shown that optimism is not only related to success, it leads to success. Optimism leads to success in academia and sports and politics. And maybe the most surprising benefit of optimism is health. If we expect the future to be bright, stress and anxiety are reduced. So all in all, optimism has lots of benefits. But the question that was really confusing to me was, how do we maintain optimism in the face of reality? As an neuroscientist, this was especially confusing, because according to all the theories out there, when your expectations are not met, you should alter them. But this is not what we find. We asked people to come into our lab in order to try and figure out what was going on. We asked them to estimate their likelihood of experiencing different terrible events in their lives. So, for example, what is your likelihood of suffering from cancer? And then we told them the average likelihood of someone like them to suffer these misfortunes. So cancer, for example, is about 30 percent. And then we asked them again, "How likely are you to suffer from cancer?" What we wanted to know was whether people will take the information that we gave them to change their beliefs. And indeed they did -- but mostly when the information we gave them was better than what they expected. So for example, if someone said, "My likelihood of suffering from cancer is about 50 percent," and we said, "Hey, good news. The average likelihood is only 30 percent," the next time around they would say,"Well maybe my likelihood is about 35 percent. " So they learned quickly and efficiently. But if someone started off saying, "My average likelihood of suffering from cancer is about 10 percent," and we said, "Hey, bad news.The average likelihood is about 30 percent," the next time around they would say, "Yep. Still think it's about 11 percent." So it's not that they didn't learn at all -- they did -- but much, much less than when we gave them positive information about the future. And it's not that they didn't remember the numbers that we gave them; everyone remembers that the average likelihood of cancer is about 30 percent and the average likelihood of divorce is about 40 percent. But they didn't think that those numbers were related to them. What this means is that warning signs such as these may only have limited impact. Yes, smoking kills, but mostly it kills the other guy. What I wanted to know was what was going on inside the human brain that prevented us from taking these warning signs personally. But at the same time, when we hear that the housing market is hopeful, we think, "Oh, my house is definitely going to double in price. " To try and figure that out, I asked the participants in the experiment to lie in a brain imaging scanner. It looks like this. And using a method called functional MRI, we were able to identify regions in the brain that were responding to positive information. One of these regions is called the left inferior frontal gyrus. So if someone said, "My likelihood of suffering from cancer is 50 percent," and we said, "Hey, good news. Average likelihood is 30 percent," the left inferior frontal gyrus would respond fiercely. And it didn't matter if you're an extreme optimist, a mild optimist or slightly pessimistic, everyone's left inferior frontal gyrus was functioning perfectly well, whether you're Barack Obama or Woody Allen. On the other side of the brain, the right inferior frontal gyrus was responding to bad news. And here's the thing: it wasn't doing a very good job. The more optimistic you were, the less likely this region was to respond to unexpected negative information. And if your brain is failing at integrating bad news about the future, you will constantly leave your rose-tinted spectacles on. So we wanted to know, could we change this? Could we alter people's optimism bias by interfering with the brain activity in these regions? And there's a way for us to do that. This is my collaborator Ryota Kanai. And what he's doing is he's passing a small magnetic pulse through the skull of the participant in our study into their inferior frontal gyrus. And by doing that, he's interfering with the activity of this brain region for about half an hour. After that everything goes back to normal, I assure you. So let's see what happens. First of all, I'm going to show you the average amount of bias that we see. So if I was to test all of you now, this is the amount that you would learn more from good news relative to bad news.Now we interfere with the region that we found to integrate negative information in this task, and the optimism bias grew even larger. We made people more biased in the way that they process information. Then we interfered with the brain region that we found to integrate good news in this task, and the optimism bias disappeared.

Learn languages from TV shows, movies, news, articles and more! Try LingQ for FREE

Tali Sharot | The optimism bias (1) ||||偏見 ||Die|| Tali Sharot|||| Tali Sharot | Optimizm meyli (1) Tali Sharot | Die Optimismus-Verzerrung (1) Tali Sharot | The optimism bias (1) Tali Sharot | El sesgo del optimismo (1) Tali Sharot | Il pregiudizio dell'ottimismo (1) タリ・シャロット|楽観主義バイアス(1) 탈리 샤롯 | 낙관주의 편향 (1) Tali Sharot | Stronniczość optymizmu (1) Tali Sharot | O viés do optimismo (1) Тали Шарот | Предвзятость оптимизма (1) Талі Шарот | Упередження щодо оптимізму (1) Tali Sharot | 乐观偏见 (1)

I'm going to talk to you about optimism -- or more precisely, the optimism bias. ben|gonna|ile|konuşmak|||hakkında|iyimserlik|ya da|daha|daha kesin olarak|bu||önyargısı It's a cognitive illusion that we've been studying in my lab for the past few years, and 80 percent of us have it. |bir|bilişsel|illüzyon|ki|biz|çalıştığımız|çalıştığımız|labda|benim|laboratuvar|son birkaç yıldır||son birkaç yıl|birkaç|yıllardır|ve|yüzde|- arasında|bizim|sahip|bu It's our tendency to overestimate our likelihood of experiencing good events in our lives and underestimate our likelihood of experiencing bad events. ||||überschätzen|||||||||||||Wahrscheinlichkeit|||| Bizim|bizim|eğilim||abartmak||olasılık||yaşama|iyi|olaylar|||hayatlarımızda||küçümsemek|||||kötü| ||||przeceniać||||||||||||||||| So we underestimate our likelihood of suffering from cancer, being in a car accident. Bu nedenle|biz|küçümsemek||||acı çekme|- kaynaklanma|||||araba|kaza We overestimate our longevity, our career prospects. |||Langlebigkeit||| |||uzun ömürl||| In short, we're more optimistic than realistic, but we are oblivious to the fact. ||||||||||ahnungslos||| ||||||||||farkında değiliz||| ||||||||||nieświadomi||| ||||||||||unaware||| Take marriage for example. In the Western world, divorce rates are about 40 percent. ||||||||yüzde That means that out of five married couples, two will end up splitting their assets. Isso significa que de cinco casais, dois acabarão dividindo seus bens. But when you ask newlyweds about their own likelihood of divorce, they estimate it at zero percent. ||||||||Wahrscheinlichkeit||||schätzen|||| ||||recently married couples|||||||||||| ||||yeni evliler|hakkında||||||||||| And even divorce lawyers, who should really know better, hugely underestimate their own likelihood of divorce. So it turns out that optimists are not less likely to divorce, but they are more likely to remarry. ||||||||||||||||wahrscheinlicher||wieder heiraten ||||||||||||||||||yeniden evlenmek ||||||||||||||||||marry again つまり、楽観主義者は離婚する確率は低くないが、再婚する確率は高いことがわかった。 Assim, os otimistas não são menos propensos a se divorciar, mas são mais propensos a se casar novamente. In the words of Samuel Johnson, "Remarriage is the triumph of hope over experience." ||||||Second marriage||||||| |||||||||zaferi|||üzerine| サミュエル・ジョンソンの言葉を借りれば、「再婚は経験に対する希望の勝利である」。 Nas palavras de Samuel Johnson, "O novo casamento é o triunfo da esperança sobre a experiência". So if we're married, we're more likely to have kids. ||evliysek|evliyiz|||||| And we all think our kids will be especially talented. This, by the way, is my two-year-old nephew, Guy. And I just want to make it absolutely clear that he's a really bad example of the optimism bias, because he is in fact uniquely talented. And I'm not alone. Out of four British people, three said that they were optimistic about the future of their own families. |||||||||olduklarını|||||||| That's 75 percent. But only 30 percent said that they thought families in general are doing better than a few generations ago. |sadece|||||||||||||||| And this is a really important point, because we're optimistic about ourselves, we're optimistic about our kids,we're optimistic about our families, but we're not so optimistic about the guy sitting next to us, and we're somewhat pessimistic about the fate of our fellow citizens and the fate of our country. |||||önemli||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||| 这是一个非常重要的观点,因为我们对自己充满乐观,对我们的孩子充满乐观,对我们的家庭充满乐观,但对坐在我们旁边的那个人并不那么乐观,对我们的同胞和国家的命运有些悲观。 But private optimism about our own personal future remains persistent. |özel|||||||devam ediyor| 但对我们自己个人未来的私人乐观态度是持久不变的。 And it doesn't mean that we think things will magically turn out okay, but rather that we have the unique ability to make it so. ||||ki|||||||||||||||||||| 这并不意味着我们认为事情会神奇地变好,而是我们有独特的能力去改变它。 Now I'm a scientist, I do experiments. ||||||deneyler So to show you what I mean, I'm going to do an experiment here with you. So I'm going to give you a list of abilities and characteristics, and I want you to think for each of these abilities where you stand relative to the rest of the population. そこで、能力と特性のリストをお見せしますので、それぞれの能力について、自分が他の人たちと比べてどのような位置にいるのかを考えてみてください。 所以我要给你列出一些能力和特点,我希望你想想对于这些能力,你在人群中处于什么位置。 The first one is getting along well with others. 第一个是和别人相处融洽。 Who here believes they're at the bottom 25 percent? |||onlar|||| Quem aqui acredita que está entre os 25% inferiores? 这里谁认为自己处在最低的25%? Okay, that's about 10 people out of 1,500. 好的,大约有1,500人中的10人。 Who believes they're at the top 25 percent? 谁相信自己在前25%? That's most of us here. 这里大部分人都是。 Okay, now do the same for your driving ability. How interesting are you? How attractive are you? How honest are you? And finally, how modest are you? So most of us put ourselves above average on most of these abilities. Now this is statistically impossible. We can't all be better than everyone else. (Laughter) But if we believe we're better than the other guy, well that means that we're more likely to get that promotion, to remain married, because we're more social, more interesting. |||||||||||||||||||||Beförderung||||||||| ||||||daha iyiyiz|||||||||||||||||||||||| And it's a global phenomenon. ||||olgu The optimism bias has been observed in many different countries -- in Western cultures, in non-Western cultures, in females and males, in kids, in the elderly. It's quite widespread. But the question is, is it good for us? So some people say no. Some people say the secret to happiness is low expectations. I think the logic goes something like this: If we don't expect greatness, if we don't expect to find love and be healthy and successful, well we're not going to be disappointed when these things don't happen.And if we're not disappointed when good things don't happen, and we're pleasantly surprised when they do,we will be happy. |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||hayal kırıklığı|||||||biz||||||||| So it's a very good theory, but it turns out to be wrong for three reasons. |||||teori|||||||||| Number one: Whatever happens, whether you succeed or you fail, people with high expectations always feel better. Because how we feel when we get dumped or win employee of the month depends on how we interpret that event. |||||||abserviert||||||||||||| ||||||alırız|terk edildiğimiz|ya da||||||||||yorumlarız|| Porque como nos sentimos quando somos dispensados ou ganhamos o funcionário do mês depende de como interpretamos esse evento. The psychologists Margaret Marshall and John Brown studied students with high and low expectations. And they found that when people with high expectations succeed, they attribute that success to their own traits. |||||||||||atfederler||||||özelliklerine "I'm a genius, therefore I got an A, therefore I'll get an A again and again in the future. “我是个天才,所以我得到了A,所以将来我会一次又一次地得到A。” " When they failed, it wasn't because they were dumb, but because the exam just happened to be unfair. ||||||||głupi||||||||| "Quando eles falharam, não foi porque eles eram burros, mas porque o exame aconteceu de ser injusto. “当他们失败时,不是因为他们笨,而是因为考试恰巧不公平。” Next time they will do better. 下次他们会做得更好。 People with low expectations do the opposite. ||||||tam tersini So when they failed it was because they were dumb,and when they succeeded it was because the exam just happened to be really easy. Então, quando eles falharam foi porque eles eram burros, e quando eles tiveram sucesso foi porque o exame aconteceu de ser muito fácil. Next time reality would catch up with them. Next time reality would catch up with them. Da próxima vez, a realidade os alcançaria. So they felt worse. Number two: Regardless of the outcome, the pure act of anticipation makes us happy. Número dois: Independentemente do resultado, o puro ato de antecipação nos deixa felizes. The behavioral economist George Lowenstein asked students in his university to imagine getting a passionate kiss from a celebrity, any celebrity. 行为经济学家乔治·洛温斯坦要求大学生想象得到任何名人的热吻。 Then he said, "How much are you willing to pay to get a kiss from a celebrity if the kiss was delivered immediately, in three hours, in 24 hours, in three days, in one year, in 10 years? |||||||||||||||||||||verildi|||||||||||||| 然后他说,“如果这个热吻能立即送达,3小时后,24小时后,3天后,1年后,或10年后,你愿意支付多少钱?” He found that the students were willing to pay the most not to get a kiss immediately, but to get a kiss in three days. 结果发现,学生们愿意支付最多的并不是立即得到热吻,而是3天后得到热吻。 They were willing to pay extra in order to wait. Now they weren't willing to wait a year or 10 years; no one wants an aging celebrity. Agora eles não estavam dispostos a esperar um ano ou 10 anos; ninguém quer uma celebridade envelhecida. But three days seemed to be the optimum amount. So why is that? Well if you get the kiss now, it's over and done with. But if you get the kiss in three days, well that's three days of jittery anticipation, the thrill of the wait. ||||||||||||||nervöser|||||| ||||||||||||||heyecanlı|||heyecan||| ||||||||||||||nerwowej|||||| The students wanted that time to imagine where is it going to happen, how is it going to happen. ||istedi|||||||||||||||| Anticipation made them happy. This is, by the way, why people prefer Friday to Sunday. It's a really curious fact, because Friday is a day of work and Sunday is a day of pleasure, so you'd assume that people will prefer Sunday, but they don't. It's not because they really, really like being in the office and they can't stand strolling in the park or having a lazy brunch. 这并不是因为他们真的非常喜欢待在办公室,无法忍受在公园漫步或悠闲地吃早午餐。 We know that, because when you ask people about their ultimate favorite day of the week, surprise, surprise, Saturday comes in at first, then Friday, then Sunday. 我们知道这一点,因为当你问人们关于他们最喜欢的一周中的一天时,惊讶的是,周六排在第一位,然后是周五,然后是周日。 People prefer Friday because Friday brings with it the anticipation of the weekend ahead, all the plans that you have. 人们喜欢周五,因为周五带来了前景,即即将到来的周末,所有你制定的计划。 On Sunday, the only thing you can look forward to is the work week. So optimists are people who expect more kisses in their future, more strolls in the park. ||||||||||||yürüyüşler||| Então, otimistas são pessoas que esperam mais beijos no futuro, mais passeios no parque. And that anticipation enhances their wellbeing. |||verbessert|| E essa antecipação aumenta o seu bem-estar. In fact, without the optimism bias, we would all be slightly depressed. People with mild depression, they don't have a bias when they look into the future. ||hafif|||||||||||| They're actually more realistic than healthy individuals. But individuals with severe depression, they have a pessimistic bias. So they tend to expect the future to be worse than it ends up being. ||eğilimindedir|||||||||||| So optimism changes subjective reality. The way we expect the world to be changes the way we see it. ||||||||||||görme şeklim| But it also changes objective reality. It acts as a self-fulfilling prophecy. ||||||kehanet And that is the third reason why lowering your expectations will not make you happy. Controlled experiments have shown that optimism is not only related to success, it leads to success. Optimism leads to success in academia and sports and politics. And maybe the most surprising benefit of optimism is health. If we expect the future to be bright, stress and anxiety are reduced. So all in all, optimism has lots of benefits. But the question that was really confusing to me was, how do we maintain optimism in the face of reality? As an neuroscientist, this was especially confusing, because according to all the theories out there, when your expectations are not met, you should alter them. Como neurocientista, isso foi especialmente confuso, porque de acordo com todas as teorias existentes, quando suas expectativas não são atendidas, você deve alterá-las. But this is not what we find. We asked people to come into our lab in order to try and figure out what was going on. We asked them to estimate their likelihood of experiencing different terrible events in their lives. So, for example, what is your likelihood of suffering from cancer? And then we told them the average likelihood of someone like them to suffer these misfortunes. |||||||||||||||Unglücke So cancer, for example, is about 30 percent. And then we asked them again, "How likely are you to suffer from cancer?" ||||||Wie||||||| What we wanted to know was whether people will take the information that we gave them to change their beliefs. And indeed they did -- but mostly when the information we gave them was better than what they expected. So for example, if someone said, "My likelihood of suffering from cancer is about 50 percent," and we said, "Hey, good news. The average likelihood is only 30 percent," the next time around they would say,"Well maybe my likelihood is about 35 percent. " So they learned quickly and efficiently. But if someone started off saying, "My average likelihood of suffering from cancer is about 10 percent," and we said, "Hey, bad news.The average likelihood is about 30 percent," the next time around they would say, "Yep. Still think it's about 11 percent." So it's not that they didn't learn at all -- they did -- but much, much less than when we gave them positive information about the future. And it's not that they didn't remember the numbers that we gave them; everyone remembers that the average likelihood of cancer is about 30 percent and the average likelihood of divorce is about 40 percent. But they didn't think that those numbers were related to them. What this means is that warning signs such as these may only have limited impact. Yes, smoking kills, but mostly it kills the other guy. What I wanted to know was what was going on inside the human brain that prevented us from taking these warning signs personally. But at the same time, when we hear that the housing market is hopeful, we think, "Oh, my house is definitely going to double in price. " To try and figure that out, I asked the participants in the experiment to lie in a brain imaging scanner. It looks like this. And using a method called functional MRI, we were able to identify regions in the brain that were responding to positive information. One of these regions is called the left inferior frontal gyrus. ||||||||||Gyri So if someone said, "My likelihood of suffering from cancer is 50 percent," and we said, "Hey, good news. Average likelihood is 30 percent," the left inferior frontal gyrus would respond fiercely. |||||||||||heftig |||||||||||şiddetle And it didn't matter if you're an extreme optimist, a mild optimist or slightly pessimistic, everyone's left inferior frontal gyrus was functioning perfectly well, whether you're Barack Obama or Woody Allen. On the other side of the brain, the right inferior frontal gyrus was responding to bad news. And here's the thing: it wasn't doing a very good job. The more optimistic you were, the less likely this region was to respond to unexpected negative information. And if your brain is failing at integrating bad news about the future, you will constantly leave your rose-tinted spectacles on. ||||||||||||||||||Rosenbrille||Sonnenbrille| So we wanted to know, could we change this? Could we alter people's optimism bias by interfering with the brain activity in these regions? ||verändern|||||||||||| And there's a way for us to do that. This is my collaborator Ryota Kanai. And what he's doing is he's passing a small magnetic pulse through the skull of the participant in our study into their inferior frontal gyrus. E o que ele está fazendo é passar um pequeno pulso magnético através do crânio do participante em nosso estudo em seu giro frontal inferior. And by doing that, he's interfering with the activity of this brain region for about half an hour. |||||zakłócając|||||||||||| E ao fazer isso, ele está interferindo na atividade dessa região do cérebro por cerca de meia hora. After that everything goes back to normal, I assure you. ||||||||sana temin eder| So let's see what happens. First of all, I'm going to show you the average amount of bias that we see. Em primeiro lugar, vou mostrar a quantidade média de viés que vemos. So if I was to test all of you now, this is the amount that you would learn more from good news relative to bad news.Now we interfere with the region that we found to integrate negative information in this task, and the optimism bias grew even larger. |||||||||||||||||||||||||haber|||||||||||||||||||||||daha büyük Então, se eu fosse testar todos vocês agora, essa é a quantidade que vocês aprenderiam mais com boas notícias em relação às más notícias. ainda maior. We made people more biased in the way that they process information. Then we interfered with the brain region that we found to integrate good news in this task, and the optimism bias disappeared. Então, interferimos na região do cérebro que descobrimos integrar boas notícias nessa tarefa, e o viés do otimismo desapareceu.